The Crack of the Bat

The crack of the bat is one of sports' most beautiful sounds. (via Keith Allison)

The crack of the bat is one of sports’ most beautiful sounds. (via Keith Allison)

For many, it is the quintessential sound of summer. It will focus the collective attention of 40,000 fans in a split second. That distinctive reverberation, the crack of the bat, has been around as long as baseball. Yet as far as I know, the physics of the crack of the bat have only been described theoretically once, and experimental data are rare.

Many fans, as well as players, will tell you a well-hit homer sounds distinctly different from a lazy fly ball. Coaches claim they can estimate the potential of a young hitter just by the sound created when his bat meets the ball.

To shed light on such claims, a friend and I headed to Phoenix for spring training in 2014. What follows is a summary of the theory behind the crack of the bat as well as a discussion of our measurements.

Adair’s Theory

In 2001, Robert Adair — renowned for The Physics of Baseball — was asked to speak on acoustics and baseball. He claimed to know “nothing of acoustics” but thoroughly explained what little theory we now have regarding the crack of the bat. His argument suggests the sound has two sources: the expulsion of air as the ball and bat collide, and the vibration of the bat after the collision is complete.

He estimates the contributions from the two are about the same size. More remarkable from my point of view, both create sounds loud enough to be heard throughout a ballpark filled with 40,000 fans.

Sound is created when air is driven out of one space and into another. The simplest example is clapping your hands. As your hands approach each other, the air in between is forced out, creating sound waves. When you bring your hands together slowly, you create a lower frequency sound compared to bringing them together rapidly. Try it. The collision time for your hands determines the frequency of the resulting sound.

The same is true for the ball-bat collision. The collision time between the ball and the bat is about 0.6 milliseconds (ms) for a well-hit ball. Lucky for you, I won’t go into the details of the theory of waves. However, it turns out that you can expect a broad collection of frequencies centered about the reciprocal of the collision time, about 1.7 kilohertz (kHz).

For a poorly hit ball, the collision time is generally longer, resulting in a collection of frequencies centered at a lower frequency. This frequency shift is a potential source of the difference in sound for a homer versus a weak ground ball.

The sound associated with the vibration of the bat is analogous to the sounds created by the vibrating strings of a guitar. When you pluck the top string, you primarily hear 82Hz. In addition, you’ll also hear – in decreasing volumes – 164 hertz (Hz), 246Hz, 328Hz, etc. These are often called the “natural frequencies” of the string.

The first four natural frequencies for a bat are known to be around 170Hz, 600Hz, 1200Hz and 2000Hz, though they vary a bit from bat to bat. For a well-hit ball, there should be relatively little excitation of these vibrational modes because they all have nodes near where a well-hit ball strikes the bat. However, for a less well-hit ball striking the end of the bat, these modes are excited to varying extents.

In summary, Adair’s theory predicts a broad group of frequencies centered at the inverse of the collision time, plus contributions from the natural vibration frequencies of the bat. For a well-hit ball, the group of frequencies should be a bit higher, and there should be little contribution from the natural frequencies of the bat. For a poorly hit ball, the group of frequencies should be a bit lower, and there should be some contribution from the natural frequencies of the bat.

The Experiment

David and Christine Vernier of Vernier Software and Technology provided us with a MCA-BTA microphone connected to a LabQuest Mini (LQ-MINI) interface that we connected to our laptop. We used Logger Pro software to record the microphone trace for hundreds of ball-bat collisions from safely outside the fence behind home plate. The accompanying software produced the frequency spectrum.

To prepare for our trip to spring training, coach Dave Taylor was kind enough to give us permission to collect some preliminary data from the California State University, Chico baseball team during their batting practice. They use non-wood bats. In this environment, the “crack of the bat” is replaced by “ping of the bat.” Figure 1 shows typical data.

CotB-1

The collision can be distinguished within the first millisecond in the upper curve. The rest of the graph is due to the vibration of the bat. There is a broad band from around 800Hz to around 1800Hz from the collision. Superimposed upon the broad band is a sharp peak a little below 1500Hz that is the dominant frequency in the ringing or the ping of the bat.

A Hardball Times Update
Goodbye for now.

While analyzing the ping of the bat was a good exercise, we were curious if the data from the crack of the bat would be significantly different. We were very fortunate to obtain permission from Steve Decker of the San Francisco Giants to place a microphone 30 feet directly behind home plate for three spring training games at their Triple-A facility in Scottsdale, Ariz. Since there was no seating for fans at these games, the sound environment was reasonably quiet.

We collected data for over 150 ball-bat collisions. We recorded the first seven milliseconds of the crack of the bat. This choice was necessitated because it was the only way to get the software to produce a frequency spectrum at 50Hz intervals. The microphone was still picking up sound at this time, presumably from the vibration of the bat. For each collision, we noted the outcome of the hit and made a qualitative judgment as to how well the ball was hit on a scale of one to five.

Some Scottsdale Data

Let me begin with the bad news. Our data is less than conclusive. On the bright side, it doesn’t clearly contradict Adair’s theory. Figures 2 and 3 are the data from two different home runs. The upper curve is the amplitude of the sounds versus time. The lower curves are the frequency distributions in the upper curve.

In the upper trace of Figure 2, the sound due to the expulsion of the air during the collision clearly can be seen within the first half millisecond or so. However, this is less clear in Figure 3. This could be because the sound in Figure 2 is louder than Figure 3.

CotB-2 CotB-3

Both spectra have substantial high-frequency components. It is unlikely they are due to bat vibrations. They might be artifacts of the method by which the software calculates the frequency graph; we simply don’t have sufficient evidence to say for sure.

The spectra both have a band of frequencies centered around 1700Hz, as predicted. They also both contain a small band centered around 300Hz that might be the fundamental vibration of the bat, and a stronger peak around 1000Hz that could be from a higher frequency mode.

Figure 4 is from a poorly hit ground ball. The frequency spectrum has peaks around 300Hz and 1000Hz like the homers. While the home runs have a band around 1700Hz, this poorly hit ball has a band centered lower at 1500Hz. This could be caused by a longer collision time, as we predicted.

CotB-4

The relative height of the 1500Hz band compared to the two lower frequency peaks is smaller than the relative height of the 1700Hz band in the homers. If these bands are due to the air expulsion, it would indicate that the poorly hit ball deposited more energy into the vibration of the bat, as one might expect.

In summary, the data we have collected using readily available, off-the-shelf equipment is less than conclusive. Nonetheless, it does demonstrate aspects of Adair’s theory.

In the future we hope to repeat the experiment with more sophisticated sound equipment. Another layer of sophistication would come from being able to use Statcast data, in order to better quantify how well a given ball was hit. Finally, it would help if we could measure the natural frequencies of the bats before we collect data. “So much to do, so little time,” said Jeremy the Nowhere Man.

References & Resources

  • Special thanks to Scott Gimbal, a CSU Chico physics major, who collected the data from the Chico baseball team batting practices, and to Paul Robinson, who helped with data collection in Phoenix.
  • Special thanks to the Vermier’s for use of their equipment, and to CSU Chico and the San Francisco Giants for allowing us to record at their facilities.
  • Robert K. Adair, Acoustical Society of America, “The crack‐of‐the‐bat: The acoustics of the bat hitting the ball,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109 , 2497 (2001).
  • Dan Russell, Physics and Acoustics of Baseball & Softball Bats, “Crack vs. Ping”
  • Alan M. Nathan, “Dynamics of the Baseball-Bat Collision,” Am. J. Phys. 68, 979-990 (2000)


David Kagan is a physics professor at CSU Chico, and the self-proclaimed "Einstein of the National Pastime." Visit his website, Major League Physics, and follow him on Twitter @DrBaseballPhD.
7 Comments
Oldest
Newest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments
Mike
8 years ago

I send praise for my favorite nerdy baseball read of 2016 so far

I like the sound a basketball makes in the net from a perfect shot as well

Publius
8 years ago

Seems odd to not reference this almost-2-year-old article which overlaps a lot with this study. As a physics professor, you should know what a “literature review” is.

http://www.baseballprospectus.com/article.php?articleid=24465

David Kagan
8 years ago
Reply to  Publius

My apologies. I had read your article as I was working out the logistics of collecting our data. Unfortunately, my memory failed me and I am glad you rectified the situation by including a link here.

Rob Arthur
8 years ago
Reply to  David Kagan

For the record, that commenter is not the author of the BP article (that would be me). I very much enjoyed your piece, David.

hopbitters
8 years ago

Interesting work. I’d love to see more/more precise data. Also, any chance you can make those graphics a little bigger?

Jetsy Extrano
8 years ago

Did you apply time-domain windowing in the computation of the spectrum? If you did not, you can get spurious high-frequency artifacts. If you do, you have to be careful it doesn’t suppress the initial crack.

For a clip 7 ms long, the sound doesn’t die away to silence. An un-windowed FFT is effectively saying “loop this clip and tell me the spectrum of that”, and that will have an artificial glitch where it loops around. A discontinuity is broadband crud sloped down at 6 dB/octave, discontinuity in first derivative is 12 dB/octave crud, etc.

This brief signal is tricky to analyze; an acoustician might have some ideas.

Jery DiMarco
7 years ago

How could you overlook something as obvious as the sound made by the 2 objects colliding? The crack of a bat hitting a ball is not from the air rushing out or the vibration of the bat. Think of what you know about the sound of rushing air, or the sound of something vibrating. Neither sound resembles a cracking sound. The obvious source of the cracking sound is the 2 hard materials colliding. This is why the crack of an aluminum bat sounds different than a wood bat. The air would not be rushing out differently for an aluminum bat. The vibration of the aluminum bat would be different, but that is a longer lasting sound which is easily distinguished from the crack sound. There is a sound made by the objects that are colliding.

Another sound source that could confuse the results is echo. There are walls, rows of seats and perhaps other equipment nearby that could be reflecting sound. One would need a better setting to get results that are more clear.

Another variable is the ball itself. Part of the sound generated is from the deformation of the ball. The sound of a deforming ball is like an instantaneous ripping sound, which lasts slightly longer than the crack of the bat. If you’ve ever played softball, you’ve heard this sound. Every time the ball is hit, it is degraded slightly. So the next time it is hit, it reacts and sounds differently. It is easier to hit a homerun off a new ball than an old ball. At batting practice, dozens of balls of varying age are in use. So this variable would have to be controlled for as well.